Academic Dishonesty and COVID-19: A Biological Explanation (2024)

by Rotem Arie

Research Paper | UWS 37a Biology of Morality | Elissa Jacobs | Spring 2021

About this paper |This paper as PDF | APA format

One of the many changes the COVID-19 pandemic brought was the shift to online learning. From pre-school to university and all around the world, education systems found themselves in a need to quickly set up a functioning remote learning system and make their faculty members and students adjust to it. A lot has been said about the pros and cons of learning online, and a lot will be said once the education world returns to its former state. But one of the side effects that has yet to be thoroughly discussed is the increase in academic dishonesty among college students who study online. A lot of universities around the world have been reporting substantially increased rates of cheating and academic dishonesty since the shift to online learning (Baskin, 2020). What might cause a drastic and global spike in academic dishonesty? Although scholars have produced numerous studies on cheating in higher education, the literature has not yet addressed the problem (or potential solutions) in the context of the current global pandemic. As a result, new questions need to be asked: what other factors can explain the increase in cheating rates? And what measures can or should be taken to decrease those rates? When the future of education is still unclear, and it appears that a component of virtual learning will continue to be a part of students’ life, understanding the reasons behind the increase in cheating might help solve the new problem. One explanation that could appear obvious is that far away from the teachers’ eyes students are less likely to get caught cheating, which essentially encourages cheating. However, research shows that this reasoning alone cannot account for the substantial increase observed by universities. What could cause such an increase? When we examine the biological literature, two less obvious factors emerge that can explain the increased cheating rate: stress and fear of loss. These factors, which are known for increasing dishonest behavior, come together at the time of a global pandemic and cause a spike in academic cheating. I will argue that rather than an expected psychological explanation like a decrease in the probability to get caught cheating, the increase in cheating in academia during the COVID pandemic is more deeply influenced. It is specifically influenced by hormonal and cognitive changes that students went through with the outburst of the pandemic, changes that were due to a combined increase in pressure and fear of loss.

According to the internet newspaper Times Higher Education, Universities all over the world have reported a significant increase in students cheating since the beginning of the COVID-19 pandemic and the shift to online learning. The University of Waterloo in Belgium reported an increase of 146% cheating cases in the last year and the University of Calgary in Canada reported a 269% increase, while Queensland University of Technology in Australia reported that test cheating quadrupled, and the University of Houston in the USA reported doubled rates (Baskin, 2020). These are just four examples and there are many more. The Kansas City Star reports an increase in cheating at the University of Missouri, North Carolina State University, Georgia Tech, Boston University, and more (Williams, 2020). The statistics show that higher-education institutions have been struggling with increasing cheating rates since the beginning of the pandemic.

Online learning is not a new concept. Even before COVID-19 spread, universities all over the world, like the University of Phoenix in the United States and the Open University of Israel, have been offering courses online. Cheating while learning online is also not a new concept. Research published in 2008 (Watson & Sottile, 2010) surveyed both undergraduate and graduate students about cheating habits they have when they take classes online vs. when they take in-person classes. The results showed that students tend to cheat more when they are being tested in person rather than when they are tested online. These results show that it is not online learning itself that encourages students to cheat, proving that the increase in cheating was caused as a result of the pandemic and was not due to the shift to online learning. What might be a possible cause for such a substantial increase in cheating rates? It is reasonable to assume that with the increase in the physical distance from the professor and the classroom, students feel like they are less likely to get caught. Social Economist Dan Ariely (2012) tested whether the probability of getting caught influences students’ drive to cheat. In the experiment, students were asked to solve a series of mathematical puzzles. The more puzzles they solved, the bigger the monetary reward they received. Some of the participants handed their results to the proctor and were rewarded accordingly, while others were asked to grade their performance themselves and then report their scores to the proctor. When the scholars then compared the results of the students who could not cheat to the results of the students who could cheat without getting caught, they found that given the opportunity, many participants chose to cheat. The results are consistent with the assumption that cheating during online learning increased since it became less detectable. Another experiment (Kajackaite & Gneezy, 2017) testing this idea asked participants to think of a number and roll dice. If the number on the dice matched the number in their head they were rewarded. Predictably, participants chose to lie in order to be rewarded. Both experiments support the assumption that students might be more inclined to cheat during exams they are taking online if they feel like they are less likely to get caught. Despite being supervised through a web camera, the teachers cannot control the surroundings or the computer screens of the students. In class, students are regularly monitored and watched and thus are less free to consult sources of information, but with the physical distance, those odds decrease, and so cheating increases.

However, the assumption that cheating increased because of a decreased cheating-detecting ability alone cannot explain the considerable increase detected by schools, as shown in another experiment that asked students to solve mathematical puzzles (Ariely, 2012). This time, the students were instructed to shred their work and take the money they deserve from a jar filled with change. Some of the students were proctored by a blind supervisor while others were proctored by a seeing one. The students who were proctored by the blind supervisor were able to take more money than they deserved without being caught, but the results of the experiment show that they cheated way less than was predicted by the researchers. These results suggest that while a smaller probability of getting caught might contribute to cheating rates, it does not solely account for the substantial increase in cheating rates during online learning.

Fear of loss

One explanation that could account for the increased cheating rates is an elevation in loss aversion. When we think about cheating, what comes to mind might be one’s unfair gain of advantage over another. Lying about your financial/medical situation in order to keep receiving welfare assistance is a common example of a situation that is perceived this way—people lying in order to gain benefits they do not deserve. Another example is thinking of corruption among politicians as their attempt to gain support and status. One famous example that was perceived as gaining an unfair advantage was the discovery that cyclist Lance Armstrong was using prohibited substances during his entire career (Thomsen & Anderson, 2015). All of these examples are perceived as the cheaters’ attempt to gain money, status, power, etc. unfairly and undeservingly. However, a different perspective shows that in contrast with conventional wisdom, some of these scenarios do not stem from the desire to gain an advantage, but rather from a fear of losing an existing one. In fact, Grolleau et al. (2016) show that fear of loss might be a more powerful driving force for cheating than a potential gain. According to them, people who lie to welfare agencies or tax officials about an improved economic state do it to avoid losing the welfare assistance they qualified for in the past. This reaction stems from a fear of losing the already existing financial benefit and not from the will to gain a non-existing advantage. Politicians do not lie to gain status; they lie in order to avoid losing a status they already possess. World-renowned cyclist Lance Armstrong used dopamine to improve his performance because of fear of losing his reputation, his career, and his world status. The researchers established that when people are facing potential loss, they are more likely to behave dishonestly than they are when they are facing a potential gain.

An experiment by Grolleau et al. (2016) also quantified the assumption that fear of loss is a stronger driving force than a potential gain when it comes to cheating. Participants were asked to solve a series of mathematical puzzles. Some participants were informed that they will be earning money for every puzzle they solve correctly, while others were given the maximum amount of money and were told that for every wrong answer money will be taken away from them. The results showed that when facing a potential loss, participants were twice as likely to cheat than participants who were earning money for their work. Furthermore, the researchers discovered that when participants believed they were not being monitored, the rates of cheating among both groups increased. The truly significant difference, however, was between the post-paid and pre-paid participants rather than the proctored and unproctored participants. The experiment shows that a fear of loss is a strong motive when it comes to dishonest behavior. During non-pandemic days, academic misconduct could potentially be driven by a desire to gain something: a good grade, a professor’s appreciation, etc., but during the global pandemic, when potential loss is amplified, the dishonest behavior will likely stem from a fear of loss rather than the desired gain. The results of the experiment show that when cheating stems from loss, the rates are substantially bigger than when cheating stems from gain. These results strengthen the assumption that fear of loss plays a key role in the substantial increase in cheating cases during COVID-19.

In order to understand why loss aversion causes such an increase in dishonest behavior, a basic understanding of the amygdala has to be established. The amygdala is found in the temporal lobe of the brain, and one of its main functions has to do with processing fear (Neurosci, 2014). When humans experience fear, the amygdala becomes active. It sends signals to other areas of the brain which can even trigger (under severe anxiety and fear) “fight or flight mode.” The amygdala is also involved in generating fear-based memories and consequently, precautions based on those memories and fear reactions. Equipped with this knowledge, a group of researchers tested the idea “that the amygdala plays a necessary role in generating loss aversion during human decision making” (Martino et al., 2010, p. 3789). The participants of the study were a group of females with amygdala lesions and a group of females without amygdala lesions. All participants were given money and were asked to play gambling games. The results showed that the participants with the amygdala lesions had a strong absence of loss-aversion, and were significantly less afraid to gamble bigger amounts of money (Martino et al., 2010). These results suggest that the amygdala and loss aversion are tightly linked, and if a correlation can be found between the amygdala and cheating, it will be possible to explain why fear of loss could potentially increase cheating rates in general and increase online academic cheating in particular.

Such a correlation has been researched by several scientists. One of the experiments was conducted by Abe et al. (2007). Participants in the experiment were asked to respond truthfully and deceptively to an interrogator’s questions. The participants also went through PET scans in order to determine their brain functions. The results showed that when the participants were deceiving the investigator, the left amygdala appeared to be active on the PET scan, drawing the researchers to the conclusion that “the left amygdala showed a significant main effect of dishonesty” (Abe et al., 2007, p. 291). These results are significant because they tie the fear of loss to dishonest behavior. It appears that dishonest behavior occurs when people experience anxiety: in this scenario, the anxiety was triggered by a sudden instruction to behave deceptively, which in turn triggered the amygdala, but as was established earlier, the amygdala can also be triggered due to an increased fear of loss. Combining the results of both experiments (Martino et al., 2010, Abe et al., 2007) shows a biological correlation between loss aversion and dishonest behavior, proving that an increase in potential loss among college students could be a factor in an increase in cheating rates.

The COVID pandemic essentially amplified a feeling of potential loss among college students. College students might be afraid of losing social connections, falling behind in class, losing money (in the form of on/off-campus employment), losing internship and career opportunities, etc. With the outburst of the pandemic, students were quickly sent home to continue their studies, and their entire surroundings transformed completely and immediately. Such a change probably caused an increased fear of loss with students being away from their friends and normal social environment, away from the usual learning atmosphere and resources they are used to, and away from their possible form of employment. In s study that surveyed students from all over the US through Instagram (Hoyt et al., 2021) students reported that the loss of income and their social life had a major influence on their mental state during the pandemic. When these results are considered with the established idea that increased fear of loss can cause a biological reaction that increases dishonest behavior, it can reasonably be assumed that one of the primary reasons colleges all over the world detected abnormally high cheating rates among their students is an increase in a fear of loss.

Stress and cheating

Loss aversion alone does not account for the elevation in cheating rates. Another contributing factor could be an increase in stress levels among students during the COVID pandemic. Stress is often linked to academic misconduct and is the subject of many studies. In one study (Ip et al., 2016) that surveyed pharmacy students in universities in North Carolina, students who admitted to cheating during their studies were asked to rate different motives of cheating. Stress was ranked at the top three motives in a list of 15 possible motives. Another study relating stress and cheating (Vengoechea et al., 2008) was done on medical students in Bogota, Colombia. The students filled a survey in which they had to indicate the level of stress they are experiencing during school. Then they were asked to approve/disapprove with statements about academic misconduct (for example, copying from another student during an exam). The results showed that the students experience above the average levels of stress in their academic environment. An overwhelming majority of the students (97.9%) did not disprove of at least one statement that supported academic misconduct, and 99.8% of the students admitted to at least one dishonest behavior during their studies. Both surveys show that students who perform under extreme pressure are highly inclined to fall into academic misconduct, which strongly supports the theory that an elevated level of stress during the COVID pandemic lead to more cheating cases.

Why does an increase in stress levels equal an increase in cheating? Biology provides multiple explanations. It appears that stress has a significant effect on the temporal lobe of the brain and, more specifically, on the hippocampus (McEwen & Sapolsky, 1995). These regions are in charge of memory and learning. Research from 1995 (McEwen & Sapolsky) shows that functioning under stress for long periods of time causes regulations in the ability of the hippocampus to retrieve and store information. These changes can be reversible or irreversible, depending on the amount of time a person is under high levels of stress. Damage in memory control due to stress can provide a possible explanation for an increase in cheating rates. Students who are not able to retain information because of increased levels of stress will be more inclined to cheat in order to get satisfactory scores. With more students feeling stressed in the presence of an unfamiliar situation, both health-wise and education-wise, it makes sense to assume they suffered some damage to their hippocampus and so to their ability to retrieve and store information, and thus started cheating more in their schoolwork.

Biology studies also suggest a correlation between the cortisol hormone and cheating. Cortisol is the body’s primary stress hormone. Under stress, more cortisol is released into the bloodstream, and its level becomes higher than it usually is (“What Is Cortisol?” 2020). Researchers (Yang et al., 2014) conducted an experiment that examined whether elevated cortisol levels would drive participants towards unethical behavior after treating them unfairly, assuming that unfair behavior is a trigger of stress. In this experiment, some of the participants were treated unfairly by the researcher. All participants had multiple opportunities to behave unethically by taking equipment lying around the lab, taking more lottery tickets than they deserved, etc. Results showed that participants who were treated unfairly tended to cheat more. Moreover, saliva tests that were taken from them before and after participation showed an elevated level of cortisol. This experiment shows a clear correlation between unethical behavior and cortisol levels in the body. Unfair treatment was used in this experiment as a stress trigger in order to elevate cortisol level, but since high cortisol levels are generated under any stressful situations, the relationship between stress and academic misconduct is clear—an elevated level of stress equals an elevated level of cheating.

Another study that supports this theory also correlates cheating to high cortisol and testosterone levels (Lee et al., 2015). The influences of testosterone on the mind include a drive to achieve status and/or resources. This drive tends to result in a variety of behaviors, among which are power/reward-seeking, risk-taking, and rule violation. Since academic cheating often includes rule violation and risk-taking, it is reasonable to assume that high testosterone levels are correlated with unethical behavior. However, it is believed that testosterone does not drive unethical behavior alone. It is the combination of testosterone and high levels of cortisol that cause such behavior. Similar to Dan Ariely’s experiment, participants in this study were asked to solve mathematical problems, report their scores, and discard their worksheets. However, this time the researchers had a way of tracking the reported results of the participants vs. their actual performance. The participants were tested for testosterone and cortisol levels and the results showed “that the association between testosterone and unethical behavior was amplified by the presence of high levels of cortisol” while “Under low cortisol, testosterone did not predict unethical behavior” (Lee et al., 2015, p. 893). It was also found that “testosterone was a marginally significant predictor of unethical behavior under high cortisol” (Lee et al., 2015, p. 893), meaning that cortisol levels are mainly in charge of the unethical behavior rather than testosterone level. This research strengthens the assumption that high levels of stress lead to high levels of cheating, strengthening the assumption that increased levels of stress among students during the pandemic could have caused such a substantial increase in academic dishonesty.

Multiple evidence proves that stress often leads to misconduct, and in a global pandemic atmosphere, stress levels are inclined to increase. The unexpected change that happened, the fear of the unknown, and the intense lockdown the students experienced, all while still handling the college workload, now in an unknown setting, were bound to elevate stress levels among students, consequently leading to an increase in academic misconduct. A survey sent to university students in Texas (Wang et al., 2020) showed that 71% of the students feel that their stress level increased during the COVID pandemic. Moreover, in her paper on academic integrity during COVID-19, Sarah Elaine Eaton (2020) from the University of Calgary in Canada, claims that “when students are under extreme stress … they can make poor choices that lead to academic misconduct.” A study conducted by Hoyt et al. (2020) surveyed random students via Instagram and concluded that students all over the United States have been reporting an increase in anxiety and stress levels. The typical levels of stress students were under during the coronavirus pandemic were further escalated as they were moved to emergency remote learning (pp. 272-73). Connecting all of the evidence shows that stress among students is a common phenomenon, and stress during the pandemic is an even more common phenomenon. Moreover, since cognitive and hormonal responses in the body that are elicited by stress ultimately drive cheating, a clear correlation can be made between all of the factors and a conclusion can be drawn: elevated stress levels among students during COVID-19 is a probable cause for increased academic dishonesty while studying remotely.

It appears that stress and fear of loss are both tightly connected when it comes to the COVID pandemic. Anxiety can lead to an increased fear of loss and an increased fear of loss can cause an increase in stress levels. Hassien et al. (2020) established that stress enhances fear by testing the reactions of mice to possible stressors, like footshocks. They also correlate their results to humans by explaining the similarity between the reaction mechanism the mice develop throughout the experiment and the symptoms people with PTSD (post-traumatic stress disorder) exhibit. Since they established a stress-enhanced fear mechanism, it is reasonable that stressed students will be more inclined to experience fear of loss. The same logic applies the other way around, as shown by the Instagram survey conducted by Hoyt et al. (2020). Surveyed college students indicate that fear of financial instability caused by loss of their student jobs and loss of social connections deeply affected their stress levels since the outbreak of the COVID pandemic. Consequently, it can be assumed that students who might experience loss will become more stressed in the face of the potential loss. This means that when we examine increased cheating rates in students during the COVID-19 era, stress and fear of loss are practically interchangeable, or at the very least have a compound effect. Both factors separately are shown to have strong effects on dishonest behavior, and together they could account for the substantial increase in academic misconduct registered in universities all over the world.

[16] Understanding that fear of loss and stress are the factors responsible for the big spike in cheating rates among students in the COVID-19 era can be the first step in fixing the problem. An initial instinct might drive us to think that increased cheating rates were due to a decrease in supervision, but scientific studies show that this is not the case. Instead, the answer lies within our biological mechanisms. Showing that the increased cheating rates stem from cognitive and hormonal changes takes some of the blame for the academic misconduct off the students. Showing that cheating is not influenced by aspects of morality (or lack thereof), but by biological reactions that happen in every human body, proves that some of the cheating cases were inevitable. In this light, it can be seen that cheating is not the real problem. Rather, cheating is the surface manifestation of a deeper problem: the elevated levels of stress and fear of loss. Consequently, solutions like increased enforcement or increased supervision might cause a further increase in already elevated stress levels which, in turn, could further increase cheating rates. Alternative solutions that might be efficient in decreasing academic misconduct during online learning will need to be related to decreasing stress levels and fear of loss levels. Possible solutions could enable students to work in groups or take open-book exams. Such solutions are likely to take some of the stress off the student while also eliminating chances of cheating. Similar to medical conditions, a true solution to the problem will come from treating the underlying disease instead of the surface symptoms.

References

Abe, N., Suzuki, M., Mori, E., Itoh, M., & Fujii, T. (2007). Deceiving Others: Distinct Neural Responses of the Prefrontal Cortex and Amygdala in Simple Fabrication and Deception with Social Interactions. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 19(2), 287–295. https://doi.org/10.1162/jocn.2007.19.2.287

Ariely, Dan (2012). The (Honest) Truth About Dishonesty, Harper Collins.

Basken, P. (2020, December 23). “Universities say student cheating exploding in Covid era.” Times Higher Education (THE). https://www.timeshighereducation.com/news/universities-say-student-cheating-exploding-covid-era

Eaton, S. E. (2020). Academic Integrity During COVID-19: Reflections from the University of Calgary. Journal of the Commonwealth Council for Educational Administration & Management.

Grolleau, G., Kocher, M. G., & Sutan, A. (2016). Cheating and Loss Aversion: Do People Cheat More to Avoid a Loss? Management Science, 62(12), 3428–3438. https://doi.org/10.1287/mnsc.2015.2313

Hassien, A. M., Shue, F., Bernier, B. E., & Drew, M. R. (2020). A mouse model of stress-enhanced fear learning demonstrates extinction-sensitive and extinction-resistant effects of footshock stress. Behavioural Brain Research, 379, 112391. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.bbr.2019.112391

Hoyt, L. T., Cohen, A. K., Dull, B., Maker Castro, E., & Yazdani, N. (2021). “Constant Stress Has Become the New Normal”: Stress and Anxiety Inequalities Among U.S. College Students in the Time of COVID-19. Journal of Adolescent Health, 68(2), 270–276. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jadohealth.2020.10.030

Ip, E. J., Nguyen, K., Shah, B. M., Doroudgar, S., & Bidwal, M. K. (2016). Motivations and Predictors of Cheating in Pharmacy School. American Journal of Pharmaceutical Education, 80(8), Article 8. https://doi.org/10.5688/ajpe808133

Kajackaite, A., & Gneezy, U. (2017). Incentives and cheating. Games and Economic Behavior, 102, 433–444. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.geb.2017.01.015

Lee, J. J., Gino, F., Jin, E. S., Rice, L. K., & Josephs, R. A. (2015). Hormones and ethics: Understanding the biological basis of unethical conduct. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 144(5), 891–897. https://doi.org/10.1037/xge0000099

Martino, B. D., Camerer, C. F., & Adolphs, R. (2010). Amygdala damage eliminates monetary loss aversion. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 107(8), 3788–3792. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.0910230107

McEwen, B. S., & Sapolsky, R. M. (1995). Stress and cognitive function. Current Opinion in Neurobiology, 5(2), 205–216. https://doi.org/10.1016/0959-4388(95)80028-X

Neurosci. (2021, April 29). Know Your Brain: Amygdala. Neuroscientifically Challenged. https://www.neuroscientificallychallenged.com/blog/know-your-brain-amygdala

Thomsen, S. R., & Anderson, H. (2015). Using the Rhetoric of Atonement to Analyze Lance Armstrong’s Failed Attempt at Redeeming His Public Image. Journal of Sports Media, 10(1), 79–99.

Vengoechea, J., Moreno, S., & Ruiz, A. (2008). Misconduct in Medical Students. Developing World Bioethics, 8(3), 219–225. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1471-8847.2007.00194.x

Wang, X., Hegde, S., Son, C., Keller, B., Smith, A., & Sasangohar, F. (2020). Investigating Mental Health of US College Students During the COVID-19 Pandemic: Cross-Sectional Survey Study. Journal of Medical Internet Research, 22(9), e22817. https://doi.org/10.2196/22817

Watson, G., & Sottile, J. (2010). Cheating in the Digital Age: Do Students Cheat More in Online Courses? Online Journal of Distance Learning Administration, 13(1). https://www.westga.edu/~distance/ojdla/spring131/watson131.html

“What Is Cortisol?” (2021, April 24). WebMD. https://www.webmd.com/a-to-z-guides/what-is-cortisol

Williams, M. R. (2021, April 24). 150 University of Missouri students caught cheating on exams held online amid COVID-19. The Kansas City Star. https://www.kansascity.com/news/local/education/article246398985.html

Yang, L.-Q., Bauer, J., Johnson, R. E., Groer, M. W., & Salomon, K. (2014). Physiological mechanisms that underlie the effects of interactional unfairness on deviant behavior: The role of cortisol activity. Journal of Applied Psychology, 99(2), 310–321. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0034413

About This Paper

Expand All

Assignment (to be added)

Video: Interview with the Author

Academic Dishonesty and COVID-19: A Biological Explanation (1)

Interview Transcript

Doug Kirshen (he him his): Hello, I'm Doug Kirshen and we're here with Rotem Arie, the author of this paper and her professor Elissa Jacobs. Welcome to you both.

Elissa Jacobs (she her): Thank you.

Doug Kirshen: I wanted to ask you first Elyssa about your course, the Biology of Morality. It is a provocative title and every semester it's one of the most popular writing seminars that everybody wants to get into. Can we start by just telling us a little bit about the course?

Elissa Jacobs: Of course. When we think about morality, we often think about a philosophical perspective or possibly a social or cultural perspective. But there's actually a significant amount of work done, especially in the past 30 or 40 years, that is starting to explore morality from a biological and evolutionary perspective. And so that is what I challenge my students to think about in this class: to find a new lens and a new perspective on the topic of morality. We asked questions about whether humans are evolved to be good to cooperate and be fair, or perhaps were evolved sometimes to be selfish and to cheat. Through this exploration, we understand humans in new ways.

In the research paper, students can pick basically any topic under the umbrella of morality, and they are tasked with understanding this topic using biology, but they are also encouraged to bring in other perspectives as well, so it's a very open topic that allows students to explore their interests.

Doug Kirshen: Rotem, Why did you pick this particular issue of morality and can you tell us about what you wrote?

Rotem Arie: Yes, I wanted to write about why students cheat more when they learn online, specifically last year during COVID. And why I chose it, I think it started with me seeing a post on social media about how cheating during exams is so much easier, and what will we do next year when we go back to study in the classroom? And I started thinking, whether or not that was true and I also remember us reading in class an article called “Why We Cheat” that was talking about when students cheat [and] why [they do] and that's how everything started, for me.

So I just started with searching for statistics, like are students really cheating more during COVID, and I found out that they were. There's a lot of statistics and papers from universities all over the world that show that people have been cheating more. Then I started reading into why they were cheating.

Doug Kirshen: You have a very impressive bibliography in this paper. Not only did you find sources on this very recent and current phenomenon, but you also found older resources that were pertinent to this. I mean COVID is new but cheating is not new. Tell us a little bit about your process of finding sources.

Rotem Arie: I started with two sources. One was the article that we were reading in class, “Why We Cheat,” and the second one was a book that was mentioned in this article, called The Honest Truth About Dishonesty. From there I found what turned out to be my counterargument. My first thought was that people are not as proctored so that's why they cheat more, but the book refutes this and shows that this is not the reason.

From there, I went on to start googling what causes people to cheat regardless of COVID situations, just like why people cheat. So I would say, maybe I went in the reverse order, found out about why people cheat in general and then narrowed myself down to COVID.

Doug Kirshen: So going into this you had a hypothesis that it was probably because people, when they were at home they don't have somebody monitoring them and making sure that they are honest. But you tested that and found out that it wasn't correct, so it gives you an opportunity to look at it from a different perspective and you learn something and then your research followed fromwhat you learned.

Rotem Arie: Yeah, exactly like that.

Doug Kirshen: Elyssa, can you tell us a little bit about what you felt Rotem did well on this paper that might be good for others to know about.

Elissa Jacobs: Absolutely, I think this paper has a lot of strengths and, big picture, that the language and the style of writing is very clear and accessible so it's very easy for the reader to follow the argument and to move through the essay. And that's not a small thing because reading something new is hard work for a reader and when the writer makes it easier through their language, through subheadings, that really helps the reader.

And I also think that the use of evidence and analysis in this essay is very well done, and taking a literature on cheating that has nothing to do with COVID and finding the connections between our current situation and how students are handling our current health crisis allows for very powerful analysis as Rotem moves between the sources and finds connections between them.

So we end up understanding COVID and cheating in this brand new way that no one else has said. This becomes a very unique argument that uses the sources to support that new claim.

One last thing that I mentioned is that the motive of this paper is strong. I think having a current event always helps us motivate our writing a little more, because it feels timely. Although it's not necessary, it's a really strong benefit here that this feels important, and so the reader gets pulled right in because this feels like it matters.

Doug Kirshen: Rotem, this may not be a fair question but I'll ask it anyway. Do you think students should be held less responsible for cheating if there are more scientific factors that lead to that happening in this situation?

Rotem Arie: I think they still should be held accountable for cheating, because that was a conscious choice they made, but I think there can be more understanding of teachers or maybe in this scenario a change of the way we teach, maybe allowing more collaborative work. ... So how do I prevent it? I understand that people are under stress and will probably be cheating more, so how can I prevent that by reducing the stress distress, or actually letting them work together.

Doug Kirshen: Do you have any advice you could offer to future students who are taking on their UWS research paper?

Rotem Arie: I would say to like your subject. I think it's really hard to write this much about something you don't like. I found that since I was so interested and invested, even if I was reading a like a really tough biological piece that I had no idea what's happening I did want to understand it. And also I did not want to be writing things I don't fully understand and be held accountable for them, because these are my words and I'm standing behind them. So I think actually liking and being interested in what I'm writing was very helpful, and also one more thing that I was keeping in mind is that in Dr. Jacobs class what we talked a lot about forming your ideas around not around sources of information, because a lot of times I would find myself writing paragraphs and each paragraph would be based on one source. And then [this] obviously does not add up to one like integrative paper, so just keeping the big picture in mind, or like building a snowman as Dr Jacobs teaches us just kind of helps you out. You know, drawing back and remembering ... what am I talking about, and not just get sucked into writing a lot about one small and maybe like not as relevant [aspect].

Academic Dishonesty and COVID-19: A Biological Explanation (2)

Doug Kirshen: You hit on something we want students to learn in the research [unit], how to keep command of your own paper. Not let the sources take over so even if you're telling us what other people said it's still encased within your context and what you want the reader to take away from that.

Elissa Jacobs: I agree, and that's something that that you do very well in this paper. We referred to it in class as being the tour guide of your paper right so that you're not sending your reader off to just discover information and connect that information on their own; you guide them through it and you're very effective at doing that in this essay.

Doug Kirshen: And you also mentioned, I want to be writing about something that I understand, and I think that's important. You could choose to [just say] I won't write about that. But you chose to figure out how to understand it. So what did you do to make that happen if you had that difficult article you mentioned biology, how did you deal with that.

Rotem Arie: So a lot of times I know we're supposed to be talking to using peer reviewed sources, but a lot of the times, I found if I didn't know some biology term just googling it [can be helpful] because a peer reviewed article can be like very high level and hard to understand. I think I have a WebMD reference in my paper for what is cortisol. Just because I felt that was easier for a person who's not from the field to understand.

And I also had to go to office hours with my biology professor and asked her about some of the things which was also very helpful to be taking a bio class at that time. But just maybe take like sentence by sentence and break it down and every word I wasn't understanding like define it for myself in simpler words. And being methodical about it and make sure that I understand, like the part that I need, and also remembering I need a very small part so don't be overwhelmed by a lot of things you don't understand; find the one little thing that you don't understand and understand that part and that's it.

Doug Kirshen: Okay, there were like 15 great points in there. ... Go to your biology professor ... why not? You have a campus full of experts and I bet ... they'd be pretty interested in having you ask that question. So ... That's a great point and and also your use of the reference sources, when we don't understand a word or term you could look it up [where it is] more accessible. [There is] science writing for the general public, so you could go and find something in you know another [not scholarly but] reliable source like the Smithsonian magazine, one of the science oriented magazines, or even like the New York Times, and then take it back to the scholarly article and see if that helps you understand more and figure out what part of it you really need to understand. Any final thoughts Elissa?

Elissa Jacobs: It was really a pleasure to watch this essay come into being through the different stages, [from] the proposal to the draft to the revision. And you should be very proud of the final product. Rotem congratulations on being in Write Now! and you have provided, something I think of really great value to other students, not only in learning about this topic, but as an example of how to do it [well]. So well done and thanks for talking to us today.

Rotem Arie: Thank you guys.

Academic Dishonesty and COVID-19: A Biological Explanation (2024)
Top Articles
Best Tips to Develop a Healthy Savings Habit
Spending money makes me extremely anxious. What can I do? 
Login Page
Skamania Lodge Groupon
How to change your Android phone's default Google account
1movierulzhd.fun Reviews | scam, legit or safe check | Scamadviser
Kristine Leahy Spouse
BULLETIN OF ANIMAL HEALTH AND PRODUCTION IN AFRICA
Think Of As Similar Crossword
6001 Canadian Ct Orlando Fl
Rainfall Map Oklahoma
Procore Championship 2024 - PGA TOUR Golf Leaderboard | ESPN
Houses and Apartments For Rent in Maastricht
Amc Flight Schedule
Char-Em Isd
Echat Fr Review Pc Retailer In Qatar Prestige Pc Providers – Alpha Marine Group
Imagetrend Inc, 20855 Kensington Blvd, Lakeville, MN 55044, US - MapQuest
Watch The Lovely Bones Online Free 123Movies
1773X To
Fort Mccoy Fire Map
Rural King Credit Card Minimum Credit Score
Mtr-18W120S150-Ul
Bj타리
Jurassic World Exhibition Discount Code
Pipa Mountain Hot Pot渝味晓宇重庆老火锅 Menu
L'alternativa - co*cktail Bar On The Pier
"Pure Onyx" by xxoom from Patreon | Kemono
Old Peterbilt For Sale Craigslist
Federal Student Aid
AsROck Q1900B ITX und Ramverträglichkeit
Giantess Feet Deviantart
Tokyo Spa Memphis Reviews
Los Garroberros Menu
Craiglist Hollywood
Taylor University Baseball Roster
Craigslist Lakeside Az
The Realreal Temporary Closure
Lamont Mortuary Globe Az
Levi Ackerman Tattoo Ideas
Suntory Yamazaki 18 Jahre | Whisky.de » Zum Online-Shop
Walmart Careers Stocker
Mountainstar Mychart Login
Dancing Bear - House Party! ID ? Brunette in hardcore action
6463896344
Jimmy John's Near Me Open
How to Do a Photoshoot in BitLife - Playbite
sin city jili
Renfield Showtimes Near Regal The Loop & Rpx
Psalm 46 New International Version
Jesus Calling Oct 6
Gameplay Clarkston
Gainswave Review Forum
Latest Posts
Article information

Author: Gregorio Kreiger

Last Updated:

Views: 6246

Rating: 4.7 / 5 (77 voted)

Reviews: 92% of readers found this page helpful

Author information

Name: Gregorio Kreiger

Birthday: 1994-12-18

Address: 89212 Tracey Ramp, Sunside, MT 08453-0951

Phone: +9014805370218

Job: Customer Designer

Hobby: Mountain biking, Orienteering, Hiking, Sewing, Backpacking, Mushroom hunting, Backpacking

Introduction: My name is Gregorio Kreiger, I am a tender, brainy, enthusiastic, combative, agreeable, gentle, gentle person who loves writing and wants to share my knowledge and understanding with you.